I'm designing a replication study. Guide me: 1) Decide on exact vs conceptual replication
This is a fantastic and crucial first step in designing a replication study! The choice between exact and conceptual replication defines your goals, methodology, and the interpretation of your results.
Let's break down each type, when to choose it, and its pros and cons.
1. Exact (Direct) Replication
Definition: An exact replication aims to reproduce the original study as closely as possible, using the same methods, materials, population characteristics, and analytical approaches. The goal is to see if the original findings can be obtained again under virtually identical conditions.
When to Choose Exact Replication:
- To Verify a Surprising or Highly Impactful Finding: If the original result is counter-intuitive, contradicts existing theory, or has significant practical/policy implications, an exact replication is essential to confirm its robustness.
- To Address Suspicions of Type I Error (False Positive): If the original study had a small sample size, borderline p-values, or used questionable statistical practices, an exact replication (especially with a larger sample) is vital.
- To Establish the Reliability of a Novel Effect: For a new phenomenon or a first-of-its-kind study, an exact replication helps confirm that the effect is indeed observable under specific conditions.
- When the Original Study is Foundational: If the original study is a cornerstone of a research area, confirming its basic findings is critical for the integrity of subsequent work.
- To Confirm Methodology: If there's doubt about the specific experimental setup or measurement, an exact replication directly tests if those specific methods yield the reported effect.
- To Assess the Reproducibility Crisis: Many exact replications are driven by the broader scientific community's effort to gauge the reproducibility of published literature.
Pros of Exact Replication:
- Direct Test of Original Finding: Clearly indicates whether the original effect is robust under its initial conditions.
- High Internal Validity: If successful, it strongly supports the validity of the original specific finding. If unsuccessful, it raises serious questions about the original.
- Clear Interpretation: Success means the effect is likely real under those conditions. Failure strongly suggests the original finding might have been a false positive or highly context-dependent.
- Builds Confidence: Confirmed exact replications enhance trust in specific published results.
- Relatively Straightforward Design: Requires meticulous attention to detail but doesn't demand new theoretical development.
Cons of Exact Replication:
- Limited Generalizability: Doesn't tell you if the effect holds in different populations, settings, or with different operationalizations.
- Doesn't Advance Theory Significantly: While it confirms specific findings, it doesn't typically extend theoretical understanding beyond the original scope.
- "Original Error" Problem: If the original study had a subtle flaw (e.g., demand characteristics, experimenter bias) that was unknowingly replicated, the replication might confirm a flawed effect.
- Can Be Difficult/Impossible: Access to original materials, specific populations, or identical equipment might be challenging or impossible. Original researchers may not share everything.
- Less "Novel" for Publication: Often perceived as less groundbreaking than conceptual replications, though this is changing with the open science movement.
2. Conceptual Replication
Definition: A conceptual replication aims to test the same theoretical idea or hypothesis as the original study, but using different methods, operationalizations, populations, or contexts. The goal is to determine if the underlying theoretical effect is robust and generalizable across different instantiations.
When to Choose Conceptual Replication:
- To Test Generalizability: If the original finding is well-established, a conceptual replication assesses if the underlying phenomenon extends to new populations, settings, or variations of the independent/dependent variables.
- To Advance Theory: By showing that a theoretical principle holds true across different operationalizations, conceptual replications strengthen and refine theories.
- To Address Methodological Limitations of the Original: If the original study had specific limitations (e.g., reliance on self-report, a specific sample, a narrow experimental paradigm), a conceptual replication can test the same idea using improved or alternative methods.
- To Explore Boundary Conditions: You might design a conceptual replication to see when and where the effect holds, and equally important, when it doesn't. This helps delineate the scope of a theory.
- When an Exact Replication is Impractical/Impossible: If you can't access the original materials, population, or setting, a conceptual replication might be the only feasible way to explore the original idea.
- To Demonstrate Robustness of an Underlying Construct: Instead of a specific operationalization, a conceptual replication focuses on the broader construct.
Pros of Conceptual Replication:
- Tests Generalizability and Robustness: Provides stronger evidence for the underlying theoretical phenomenon across diverse contexts.
- Advances Theory: Helps refine, extend, or specify the boundary conditions of a theory.
- Addresses Methodological Nuances: Less susceptible to the specific methodological quirks or errors of the original study.
- More "Novel" for Publication: Often seen as contributing new insights and therefore potentially more publishable.
- More Flexible Design: Allows for creative solutions and adaptations.
Cons of Conceptual Replication:
- Difficult Interpretation of Failure: If a conceptual replication fails, it's harder to pinpoint why. Did the original theory fail, or did your new operationalization simply miss the mark, or was there an issue with your specific design?
- **Lower Internal Validity Check of Original Specific Finding:** It doesn't directly verify the original study's specific result under its original conditions.
- Requires More Design Expertise: Designing a conceptually equivalent but methodologically different study can be challenging.
- Risk of "Straw Man" Argument: If the conceptual replication diverges too much, critics might argue it's not truly replicating the original idea.
- Needs Careful Justification: You must clearly articulate why your new methods are conceptually equivalent to the original's.
Key Decision Factors for Your Study:
To help you decide, ask yourself the following questions:
- What is my primary goal?
- Is it to confirm a specific finding? (Exact)
- Is it to extend or generalize a theoretical idea? (Conceptual)
- Is it to check the validity of a novel or surprising finding? (Exact)
- Is it to improve upon the methodology of an existing finding? (Conceptual)
- How confident are we in the original finding?
- Is it a brand-new, never-before-seen effect? (Leans Exact first)
- Is it a well-established effect, but you want to see if it holds in a new context? (Leans Conceptual)
- Are there reasons to doubt the original's statistical power or methodology? (Leans Exact)
- What resources do I have?
- Can I access the original materials, code, and exact population? (Facilitates Exact)
- Do I have the expertise and time to design entirely new but theoretically equivalent methods? (Facilitates Conceptual)
- What is the impact of the original finding?
- Does it have policy implications? Is it highly controversial? (Strong argument for Exact)
- Is it a foundational piece of theory that needs broad empirical support? (Leans Conceptual, after initial exact verification if needed)
Recommendation: Often, a Hybrid Approach or Sequential Steps
It's common (and often ideal) to pursue both:
- Start with an Exact Replication: Especially for a novel or surprising finding. This establishes the basic phenomenon under its initial conditions.
- Follow with Conceptual Replications: Once the effect is confirmed to be robust under original conditions, then you can start varying aspects (population, context, operationalization) to test its generalizability and theoretical boundaries.
**If you must choose one for your current study:**
- Choose Exact if your priority is to verify the original specific result as a baseline, especially if you have doubts about its initial robustness or if it's a very new finding.
- Choose Conceptual if the original finding is reasonably well-established and your priority is to explore its broader theoretical implications, generalizability, or refine the underlying theory.
Regardless of your choice, pre-registering your study design and analysis plan is crucial for increasing transparency and credibility!
Good luck with your replication study design!
When designing a replication study, one of the first decisions you need to make is whether to conduct an exact replication or a conceptual replication. Here’s a guide to help you decide:
1. Exact Replication
Definition:
Exact replication involves repeating the original study as closely as possible, using the same methods, materials, procedures, and conditions.
Pros:
- Direct Comparison: Allows for a direct comparison with the original study, making it easier to determine if the original findings are robust and reliable.
- Methodological Rigor: Ensures that any differences in results are due to random variation rather than methodological differences.
- Simplicity: Often simpler to design and execute because you are following a pre-established protocol.
Cons:
- Limited Generalizability: May not address whether the findings generalize to different contexts or populations.
- Resource Intensive: Can be resource-intensive if the original study was complex or required specialized equipment or settings.
When to Choose:
- When the original study's findings are controversial or have significant implications.
- When you want to validate the original study's methods and results.
- When the original study is relatively straightforward and feasible to replicate exactly.
2. Conceptual Replication
Definition:
Conceptual replication involves testing the same theoretical hypothesis but using different methods, materials, procedures, or conditions.
Pros:
- Generalizability: Helps to determine whether the findings generalize across different contexts, populations, or methodologies.
- Robustness: Provides stronger evidence for the theoretical hypothesis if the findings are consistent across different methods.
- Flexibility: Allows for innovation and adaptation, which can be useful if the original study had limitations or if you want to explore the hypothesis in a new context.
Cons:
- Complexity: Can be more complex to design and execute because you need to ensure that the new methods are valid and comparable to the original study.
- Interpretation Challenges: Differences in results may be harder to interpret because they could be due to methodological differences rather than the hypothesis itself.
When to Choose:
- When you want to test the robustness and generalizability of the original findings.
- When the original study had limitations or methodological issues that you want to address.
- When you are interested in exploring the hypothesis in a different context or with a different population.
Decision-Making Process
- Review the Original Study:
- Understand the methods, materials, and procedures used.
- Identify any limitations or methodological issues.
- Assess Feasibility:
- Determine whether you have the resources (time, money, expertise) to conduct an exact replication.
- Consider whether the original study's methods are still relevant and applicable.
- Evaluate the Goals:
- Clarify your research goals. Are you aiming to validate the original findings or to explore their generalizability?
- Consider the implications of your findings. Will they have practical applications or theoretical significance?
- Consult with Experts:
- Seek advice from experts in the field to discuss the pros and cons of each approach.
- Consider collaborating with the original researchers if possible.
- Develop a Plan:
- Outline the steps for your replication study, including the methods, materials, and procedures you will use.
- Ensure that your plan is feasible and aligns with your research goals.
By carefully considering these factors, you can make an informed decision about whether to conduct an exact or conceptual replication study.
💡 Try this comparison yourself:Compare AI models side-by-side on SNEOS
Analysis
This comparison demonstrates the different approaches each AI model takes when responding to the same prompt. Here are the key differences observed:
Response Characteristics
Gemini: Provides a detailed response with 91 sentences.
Mistral: Provides a direct response with 39 sentences.
Key Takeaways
- Each model brings unique strengths to this type of query
- Response styles vary significantly between models
- Consider your specific use case when choosing between these models
Try This Comparison Yourself
Want to test these models with your own prompts? Visit SNEOS.com to compare AI responses side-by-side in real-time.
This comparison was generated using the SNEOS AI Comparison ToolPublished: October 15, 2025 | Models: Gemini, Mistral